Skip to main content
  • Original research
  • Open access
  • Published:

Investigating sex differences and age of onset in emotion regulation, executive functioning, and cannabis use in adolescents and young adults

Abstract

Background

Young adults have historically high levels of cannabis use at a time which coincides with emotional and cognitive development. Age of regular onset of cannabis use and sex at birth are hypothesized to influence the relationship between cannabis use and cognition. Here we investigated past 6-month cannabis use in relation to emotional and executive functioning. We further considered age of onset and sex in subgroup analyses.

Method

Young adults (N = 225; ages 16–22) completed a substance use interview and cognitive battery, including the Emotional Word-Emotional Face Stroop and NIH toolbox executive functioning tasks. Linear regressions examined relationships between past 6-month cannabis use episodes and performance. Subgroup analyses investigated whether age of onset or sex impacted relationships.

Results

After correcting for multiple comparisons, greater past 6-month cannabis use episodes were related to poorer Emotional Stroop Congruent Accuracy (p = .0004, FDR-p = .002) and List Sorting Working Memory (p = .02, FDR-p = .10) performance. Younger age of regular use onset marginally related to lower Emotional Stroop Congruent Accuracy performance (p = .03, FDR-p = .13). There were no cannabis use by sex interactions on cognition.

Conclusions

Consistent with prior findings, results suggest small reductions in cannabis-related performance in processing speed during emotional Stroop and working memory tasks. Age of onset was modestly related to Stroop performance, but not sex. Longitudinal studies which detail patterns of cannabis and other substance use are needed to better assess brain-behavior relationships and other factors (e.g., age of onset of regular use, sex) which could influence cannabis-related impairments in cognitive functioning.

Introduction

Adolescent and young adult cannabis use is associated with frontolimbic brain circuit disruptions as evidenced by decreased neural connectivity and reduced white matter tissue integrity in individuals with regular cannabis use [1,2,3,4]. These regions underly reward pathways and are crucial for effective neurocognitive functioning, including emotional control and executive functioning [5]. Yet the association between cannabis and these higher order cognitive functions is not always consistent [6,7,8]. Given that cannabis use reached peak prevalence rates among 18-30-year-olds in 2021 [9], understanding how cannabis is associated with cognitive outcomes and subgroups (e.g., by sex, age of onset) that may influence cannabis-cognition relationships in emerging adults is important, particularly in an evolving and growing cannabis product market.

Neurodevelopment, including structural and functional brain maturation, persists through the third decade of life [10, 11]. These changes include connections between prefrontal and limbic (i.e., frontolimbic) regions, which are key for emotional development and reward system processing [12]. Cannabis use during this time has been linked frontolimbic disruptions [1, 3, 13] and, more broadly, to a range of executive dysfunction [14, 15] and emotional control deficits [13, 16, 17], though a recent review of this topic area suggests smaller-scale reductions in cognition in the absence of persistent and heavy cannabis use during adolescence [18]. Notably, the majority of work in this area has utilized neuroimaging to indirectly assess emotional control, therefore it is important to assess the downstream behavioral impact of cannabis use in this domain as well as prefrontal functioning more broadly through executive functioning.

Several behavioral and biological factors are postulated to relate to decrements in cognition in those with cannabis use, which may partially explain prior inconsistent findings. Age of regular cannabis use onset (e.g., weekly use) has been linked to executive dysfunction [19] and poorer planning abilities [20]. Females are purported to have a particular vulnerability to cannabis use [21] during neurodevelopment that is related to worse performance on attention and executive functioning tasks [16] and altered functional connectivity during affective processing [13], amongst other cognitive domains like memory [22]. This may be due to females exhibiting greater acute cannabis-use effects in both high and cannabinoid concentration [23]. While female sex and young age of initiation are routinely thought to convey heightened risk to the deleterious effects of cannabis use on neurodevelopment, these risk factors are not consistently studied in emerging adult cannabis studies.

Given the evidence for frontolimbic disruption in those who use cannabis, here we aim to investigate emotional control using a newly validated Emotional Stroop task [24] and executive functioning tasks from the NIH Toolbox in adolescent and young adults who use cannabis. Regression analyses will examine whether past 6-months cannabis use relates to cognitive performance across tasks. We further include consideration of other important potential factors in cognitive performance and cannabis use: namely, we group by age of onset of regular cannabis use and provide comparisons by sex.

Methods

Participants

Participants from an ongoing study in San Diego, California, were included in the present analyses. They were recruited via flyers posted around local colleges and universities and through online advertising. Interested participants called a laboratory phone for verbal consenting and screening to assess eligibility. Eligible participants included 129 participants ages 16–22 who endorsed current, regular (at least weekly) cannabis use and 96 participants of the same age who did not use cannabis regularly (< weekly) in the past six months. Nicotine and tobacco product (NTP) and alcohol use were assessed across both those who use cannabis and controls.

Exclusion criteria

Participants were excluded if they reported: excessive prenatal alcohol (maternal use of > 2 drinks per occasion, > 4 drinks in a week), tobacco, or drug exposure; premature birth (< 34 weeks gestation); other gestational or perinatal complications, including low birth weight (< 5 lbs); history of serious medical or neurological problems; head trauma with loss of consciousness > 2 min; current or past DSM-5 diagnoses other than cannabis or nicotine use disorder; learning disability; current use of psychotropic medications; non-correctable vision/hearing difficulties; not fluent in English; pregnancy; use of alcohol or cannabis within 12 h of study visit which would indicate potential current intoxication [25,26,27].

Procedures

Eligible participants attended a 4-hour in-person study session which included cognitive testing, substance use interviews, and magnetic resonance imaging (data presented elsewhere [28, 29]). Participants were abstinent from all drugs other than nicotine on the day of the appointment (i.e., 12 h of required abstinence), with abstinence confirmed via toxicological testing. Participants gave written informed consent accordance with the University of California, San Diego Human Research Protections Program.

Measures

Neurocognition

Emotional stroop

Participants completed the Emotional Word-Emotional Face Stroop (EWEFS) [24], in which participants were presented with emotional words overlaid on emotional faces that were either congruent or incongruent. They then sorted stimuli (emotional words) as either “good” or “bad”, while ignoring the image presented in the background. Four outcomes were measured: Congruent Accuracy, the proportion of congruent blocks that were accurately categorized; Incongruent Accuracy, the proportion of incongruent blocks that were accurately categorized; Congruent Response Time, the mean time (in milliseconds) to respond to congruent trials; Incongruent Response Time, the mean time (in milliseconds) to respond to incongruent trials. NIH Toolbox. For tests of executive functioning, the National Institutes of Health (NIH) Toolbox Cognition Battery [30] was used. Participants completed tasks on 3rd generation iPad Air devices (10.5in) under the administration of a trained research assistant. For the present study, included tasks measured domains of executive functioning: for set-shifting, participants completed the Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS) Test, a card sorting task wherein participants had to respond to changing rules; for inhibitory control and attention, they completed the Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention Test, wherein participants responded to a displayed central target in the midst of a row of stimuli; for working memory, they completed the List Sort Working Memory Test, where they sorted audible and visual stimuli from smallest to largest; for episodic memory, participants completed the Picture Sequence Memory Test, wherein they recalled the sequence of visually displayed images; for processing speed, they completed the Pattern Comparison Process Speed Test, wherein participants quickly identified whether or not two images were identical. Uncorrected scores were used in the present analyses.

Substance use history

Research assistants administered a modified version of the original Customary Drinking and Drug Use Record (CDDR) [31,32,33,34] to participants to collect detailed past year and lifetime substance use history. Substance “use episodes” constituted the primary variables of interest. Multiple use episodes of substances were logged if a participant had used more than once per day (e.g., in the morning, class in the afternoon, and before bed would equate to three use episodes). Additionally, other substance use variables of interest including self-report of age of onset of regular cannabis use and average cannabis use potency (for flower, reported as Low (< 5% THC), Medium, (10%), High (15%), Very High (20%+), or Don’t Know; for concentrate, Low (around 20% THC), Medium (40%), High (60%), Very high (80%+), or Don’t Know) were also collected. Age of onset of regular use was defined as the age at which a participant regularly (> weekly) used cannabis for at least a year.

Toxicological assessment

Urine, oral fluid, and breathalyzer samples for alcohol were collected and tested to confirm self-report and abstinence from cannabis, alcohol, and other drugs. However, participants were allowed to use NTP as needed to prevent withdrawal effects.

Sociodemographics

Participants self-reported sociodemographic characteristics (sex, race, ethnicity, education, maternal education as an estimate of socioeconomic status). While all were considered for inclusion in analyses, only sociodemographic factors that differed by past-month cannabis use status were included as covariates (i.e., sex at birth and race; see Sociodemographics in Results below).

Statistical analyses

Groups and subgroups

Cannabis use groups were created and used only to identify covariates; primary analyses were run with cannabis use as a continuous variable. The cannabis use group was defined as those who regularly used (> weekly, or > 24 use episodes) cannabis over the past 6 months (range of use: 28 − 3,557 episodes); controls were those who did not regularly use cannabis (< weekly) in the past 6 months (range of use: 0–22 episodes). Additionally, subgroups were created and used in secondary analyses to assess the influence of age of onset of regular use or sex in relation to cannabis-cognition relationships. Age of Onset was defined as the first age at which an individual engaged in regular (> weekly for at least a year) cannabis use [35]. Age of onset subgroups consisted of those who had never regularly used cannabis (n = 98), those who regularly used at or after age 18 (n = 76), and those who regularly used before age 18 (n = 51), as 18 has previously been used as a cut-off for age of regular onset [35]. Participants were also separately assessed by sex at birth in order to determine whether cognition-cannabis relationships may be sex-dependent.

Primary analyses

All analyses were run in R version 4.1.0 [36] via RStudio [37]. Linear regression models using the “stats” package investigated past 6-months cannabis use (continuous use variable) in relation to emotional and executive functioning. Summary scores for each cognitive task were run in nine separate regressions: Emotional Stroop Congruent Accuracy, Emotional Stroop Congruent Response Time, Emotional Stroop Incongruent Accuracy, Emotional Stroop Incongruent Response Time, Card Sort, Flanker, List Sorting, Picture Sequencing, and Pattern Comparison. Covariates. Sociodemographic factors that differed by cannabis use group status were included as covariates (i.e., race, sex) as well as age, given the age range of participants and definition of age of onset of regular use. All analyses were run with cannabis use as a continuous variable by regression with past 6-month cannabis use predicting cognitive performance; no groups were used in the primary analyses. In addition to race and sex, past 6-month nicotine use episodes and past 6-month alcohol use episodes were combined and included as a continuous covariate representing history of other substance use. Multiple comparisons were adjusted within models using the “sjstats” package for false-discovery rate corrections. Data were checked for outliers with Cook’s d, with no outliers revealed. Statistical decisions were made if corrected p (FDR-p) < 0.05.

Secondary analysis

Age of onset of regular cannabis use and sex at birth were separately considered in independent subgroup regressions. Subgroups were defined as listed in the Groups and Subgroups section above. Linear regression analyses were run assessing onset subgroup [no onset (never regular user) v. early onset (regular use before age 18) v. late onset (regular use at or after 18 years-old)] in relation to cognition, controlling for past 6-months combined nicotine and alcohol use, race, sex, and age. For analyses on sex-based effects, interaction terms between past 6-month cannabis use and sex were run in linear regressions, controlling for past 6-months nicotine and alcohol use, race, and age. Multiple comparisons were adjusted within models using the “sjstats” package for false-discovery rate corrections. Statistical decisions were made if corrected p (FDR-p) < 0.05.

Post-hoc correlations

Given that recency and potency of cannabis use may influence cognitive performance, Pearson correlations were run between self-reported days since last episode of cannabis use and self-reported potency with each cognitive task. For those reporting on potency, when there was discrepant potency between flower and concentrate, the higher potency product was included; in addition, reports of “don’t know” for potency were excluded from analyses. As not all participants had used cannabis, the for rency of use was n = 184 and for potency was n = 150.

Results

Sociodemographics and substance use history

Full sociodemographic and substance use characteristics are displayed in Table 1. Mean age of participants was 19.5 years. Of the n = 79 participants who did not use cannabis regularly in the past 6-month, n = 41 had never used cannabis. Out of all participants, 48% had used cannabis flower that they believed was at least 15% THC in potency. To determine covariates, participants were divided into a regular cannabis using group and controls. Groups differed by sex (χ2=8.36, p = .004) and race (Fisher’s two-tailed p = .03), but not by any other sociodemographic characteristics.

Table 1 Sociodemographics and substance use characteristics by current, regular cannabis use status

Primary analyses

Emotional Stroop

Regression analyses assessed past 6-month cannabis use episodes as a continuous variable in relation to four aspects of Emotional Stroop performance, controlling for nicotine use and alcohol use episodes, sex, and race; group status was not included in regressions. Past 6-month cannabis use was negatively associated with Congruent Trials Accuracy after correcting for multiple comparisons, such that greater cannabis use was associated with decreased accuracy (b = -0.00001, t = -3.56, p = .0004, FDR-p = .002; see Fig. 1). Congruent response time, incongruent accuracy, and incongruent response time were not significantly related to cannabis use.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Scatterplot of Cannabis Use predicting Congruent Trial Accuracy. Greater past-6-month cannabis use negatively predicted decreased Emotional Stroop Congruent Trials Accuracy (percent of trials correct)

Executive functioning

Greater past 6-month cannabis use was marginally associated with poorer List Sorting performance after correcting for multiple comparisons (b=-0.005, t=-2.33, p = .02, FDR-p = .10; see Fig. 2). There were no significant results for the models predicting Card Sorting, Picture Sequencing, or Pattern Comparison performance (p’s > 0.05).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Scatterplot of Cannabis Use predicting List Sorting Performance. Greater past-6-month cannabis use marginally, negatively predicted decreased List Sort performance (standard score)

Secondary analyses

Age of onset of regular cannabis use

There were no significant associations between age of onset group and any of the nine scores for neurocognitive tasks. Prior to correcting for multiple comparisons, onset group significantly predicted Congruent Trials Accuracy performance (b = -0.004, t = -2.24, p = .03, FDR-p = .13), such that those who had regular onset of cannabis before age 18 demonstrated the poorest performance.

Sex at birth

No sex by past 6-month cannabis interactions were found to be significantly predictive of neurocognitive performance on any task.

Post-hoc correlations

Correlations were run between days since last cannabis use (n = 184) and range of potency of THC (n = 150) with cognitive performance across tasks. No significant correlations were revealed.

Discussion

The present study aimed to assess the relationship between cannabis use and emotional control and executive functioning in adolescents and young adults. Relationships emerged between increased past 6-month cannabis use and poorer accuracy on the Emotional Stroop and List Sorting tasks, though findings were modest after correction for multiple comparisons. Hypothesized relationships between age of onset of regular cannabis use with Emotional Stroop were similarly modest after corrections, and no relationships were significant with sex at birth and any task performance .

Here we used a newly validated Emotional Stroop paradigm [24] to test emotional functioning in emerging adults with cannabis use. This task jointly assesses behavioral inhibition, cognitive control, and emotional control. We found effects of past-6-month cannabis use relating to decreased congruent accuracy after correcting for multiple comparisons, indicating potential decrements in processing speed and attention during emotional processing associated with use. This is consistent with prior results in an overlapping yet different and smaller sample, where we similarly found decreased accuracy on the Stroop with increased cannabis use, though that study aimed to investigate the influence of NTP use [38]. Only one of four measures from the Emotional Stroop had demonstrated a significant relationship. This may suggest that emotional processing abilities, at least as assessed within this task, are less susceptible to cannabis use than anticipated from prior research on frontolimbic functioning in typically developing and medically healthy samples [1, 3, 13, 16, 17]. Notably, congruent accuracy performance is thought to measure processing speed, rather than pure emotional functioning [24]. As this task has only been recently validated as part of the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study [24], it may also be that this task does not tap into socioaffective processing in young adults with cannabis use as sensitively as in largely substance naïve adolescents. This may also be supported by the modest relationships demonstrated between those younger age of regular use onset. However, the tasks may show more robust associations with substance use in those with heavier and more chronic use over the lifespan. Continued investigation into this and other tasks, particularly longitudinally, are needed to clarify cannabinoid-socioemotional functioning relationships.

List sorting performance was modestly associated with past-6-month cannabis use. The list sorting task was designed to tap into working memory abilities [39]—a construct frequently found to have cannabis-related deficits [40]. The endocannabinoid system is through to play an important role in modulating memory [41] due to the high density of CB1 receptors present in the prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, anterior cingulate, and cerebellum [42]. Thus, endogenous cannabis use likely impacts memory functioning and working memory abilities [43, 44]. Other executive functioning tasks were not associated with cannabis use; several reasons for this are considered. First, as summarized in a recent review [18], findings of modest decrements in select domains of cognitive functioning related to cannabis use in the developing brain are common. Importantly, though participants here included regular cannabis users, they may not be using typical products, as less than half of participants used cannabis with above average THC potency (15% THC as determined from seized samples, [45]). In addition, the present study used the NIH Toolbox, which is a validated research battery for neurocognitive assessment. Prior research has utilized clinical batteries (e.g., 14, 15), and other research in substance-using populations has suggested the NIH Toolbox may not be a sensitive battery for detecting substance and cognition relationships [46].

The secondary aim of the present analyses was to consider two commonly hypothesized factors of risk in brain-behavior relationships in cannabis use—age of onset of regular cannabis use and sex. However, despite predictions, there were no significant relationships between onset and sex subgroups and cognitive outcomes after corrections for multiple comparisons. For age of onset, this is in line with a systematic review that found only occasional age-dependent effects in both human and rodent samples [47]. For sex, it is intriguing that no sex differences in brain-behavior relationships emerged after corrections for multiple comparisons, despite a growing body of research indicating a range of sex-specific cognitive deficits in cannabis users [16, 22, 48, 49]. It may be that emerging adults with regular cannabis use are already outside the window of the highest vulnerability to the influence of cannabis rather than younger adolescents. Alternatively, while cannabis use was analyzed continuously, participants may be relatively light users early in their use trajectory (i.e., not using the majority of days in the past month) and therefore differences may still emerge with more chronic and longstanding use. Finally, other analyses from this sample have revealed largely intact white matter microstructure and cerebral blood flow [28, 29] as related to cannabis use, indicating that there may not be structural changes in these users that would warrant related functional changes in these youth still early in their use trajectories; however, this needs to be investigated directly.

Limitations to this study are noted. As described above, cannabinoid potency is rapidly changing [45] and may influence cognitive outcomes; however, here we relied on self-reported potency use and considered potency limitedly within this study. Participants may be using cannabis that is lower in THC potency than typically used in other samples. We also did not query cannabidiol (CBD) use, which may be another important factor in cannabinoid-brain-behavior relationships. While our investigation did not extend beyond the past 6 months of cannabis use, we acknowledge the significance of future research examining cannabis use trajectories, especially those individuals who exhibit patterns of heavy use followed by cessation [6]. Cannabis-use groups were used only to select covariates, with primary analyses conducted using episodic cannabis use information in linear regressions; secondary analyses used grouping variables (age of onset, sex) as interaction terms with continuous cannabis use information. We did not investigate cannabis use beyond past 6-months, though it is important for future research to consider cannabis use trajectories and, particularly, those who use heavily then cease use. As noted, neurocognitive measures were limited to more research-based assessments, which may not be as sensitive for changes in cognitive performance. Though MRI metrics were collected for many of these participants, they were not assessed here due to power concerns; future analyses will focus analysis on multimodal imaging metrics in conjunction with neurocognitive performance in this sample. Finally, findings may not generalize to other populations.

Taken together, we found modest evidence of associations between slower processing speed in an Emotional Stroop task (including in those with earlier age of regular use onset) and poorer working memory associated with more past 6-month cannabis use in adolescents and young adults. Yet, we found no other cannabis-cognition relationships, nor did we find other significant relationships by age of onset of regular cannabis use or sex at birth. Longitudinal studies which detail patterns of cannabis and other substance use are needed to better assess brain-behavior relationships and test the role that other factors (e.g., age of regular use, sex) play in these relationships.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. Shollenbarger S, Thomas AM, Wade NE, Gruber SA, Tapert SF, Filbey FM, et al. Intrinsic Frontolimbic connectivity and mood symptoms in young adult cannabis users. Front Public Health. 2019;7:311.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Wade NE, Thomas AM, Gruber SA, Tapert SF, Filbey FM, Lisdahl KM. Binge and cannabis co-use episodes in relation to white matter integrity in emerging adults. Cannabis Cannabinoid Res. 2020;5(1):62–72.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Shollenbarger SG, Price J, Wieser J, Lisdahl K. Poorer frontolimbic white matter integrity is associated with chronic cannabis use, FAAH genotype, and increased depressive and apathy symptoms in adolescents and young adults. Neuroimage Clin. 2015;8:117–25.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Zhou F, Zimmermann K, Xin F, Scheele D, Dau W, Banger M, et al. Shifted balance of dorsal versus ventral striatal communication with frontal reward and regulatory regions in cannabis-dependent males. Hum Brain Mapp. 2018;39(12):5062–73.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Luciana M. Risks versus consequences of adolescent and young adult substance use: a focus on executive control. Curr Addict Rep. 2020;7(4):453–63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Scott JC, Slomiak ST, Jones JD, Rosen AFG, Moore TM, Gur RC. Association of cannabis with cognitive functioning in adolescents and young adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. 2018;75(6):585–95.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Lisdahl KM, Wright NE, Kirchner-Medina C, Maple KE, Shollenbarger S. Considering cannabis: the effects of regular cannabis use on neurocognition in adolescents and young adults. Curr Addict Rep. 2014;1(2):144–56.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Wade NE, Kaiver CM, Wallace AL, Hatcher KF, Swartz AM, Lisdahl KM. Objective aerobic fitness level and neuropsychological functioning in healthy adolescents and emerging adults: Unique sex effects. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2020;51:101794.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Patrick ME, Schulenberg JE, Miech RA, Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG. Monitoring the Future Panel Study annual report: National data on substance use among adults ages 19 to 60, 1976–2021. Monitoring the Future Monograph Series. In: Institute for Social Research TUoM, editor. Ann Arbor. 2022.

  10. Gogtay N, Giedd JN, Lusk L, Hayashi KM, Greenstein D, Vaituzis AC, et al. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. PNAS. 2004;101(21):8174–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Giedd JN, Raznahan A, Alexander-Bloch A, Schmitt E, Gogtay N, Rapoport JL. Child psychiatry branch of the National Institute of Mental Health longitudinal structural magnetic resonance imaging study of human brain development. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40(1):43–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Casey BJ, Heller AS, Gee DG, Cohen AO. Development of the emotional brain. Neurosci Lett. 2019;693:29–34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Sullivan RM, Maple KE, Wallace AL, Thomas AM, Lisdahl KM. Examining inhibitory affective Processing within the Rostral Anterior Cingulate Cortex among Abstinent Cannabis-using adolescents and young adults. Front Psychiatry. 2022;13:851118.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Infante MA, Nguyen-Louie TT, Worley M, Courtney KE, Coronado C, Jacobus J. Neuropsychological Trajectories Associated with adolescent alcohol and Cannabis Use: a prospective 14-Year study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2020;26(5):480–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Wade NE, Wallace AL, Swartz AM, Lisdahl KM. Aerobic Fitness Level Moderates the Association between Cannabis Use and Executive Functioning and Psychomotor Speed following abstinence in adolescents and young adults. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2019;25(2):134–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Savulich G, Rychik N, Lamberth E, Hareli M, Evins AE, Sahakian BJ, et al. Sex differences in neuropsychological functioning are domain-specific in adolescent and young adult regular cannabis users. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2021;27(6):592–606.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Aloi J, Blair KS, Crum KI, Meffert H, White SF, Tyler PM, et al. Adolescents show differential dysfunctions related to alcohol and cannabis use disorder severity in emotion and executive attention neuro-circuitries. Neuroimage Clin. 2018;19:782–92.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Scott JC. Impact of adolescent cannabis use on neurocognitive and brain development. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2023;32(1):21–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Fontes MA, Bolla KI, Cunha PJ, Almeida PP, Jungerman F, Laranjeira RR, et al. Cannabis use before age 15 and subsequent executive functioning. Br J Psychiatry. 2011;198(6):442–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Becker MP, Collins PF, Schultz A, Urosevic S, Schmaling B, Luciana M. Longitudinal changes in cognition in young adult cannabis users. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2018;40(6):529–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Nia AB, Mann C, Kaur H, Ranganath M. Cannabis use: neurobiological, behavioral, and Sex/Gender considerations. Curr Behav Neurosci Rep. 2018;5:271–80.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Crane NA, Schuster RM, Mermelstein RJ, Gonzalez R. Neuropsychological sex differences associated with age of initiated use among young adult cannabis users. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2015;37(4):389–401.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Sholler DJ, Strickland JC, Spindle TR, Weerts EM, Vandrey R. Sex differences in the acute effects of oral and vaporized cannabis among healthy adults. Addict Biol. 2021;26(4):e12968.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Smolker HR, Wang K, Luciana M, Bjork JM, Gonzalez R, Barch DM, et al. The emotional Word-Emotional Face Stroop task in the ABCD study: psychometric validation and associations with measures of cognition and psychopathology. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2022;53:101054.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Dahlgren MK, Sagar KA, Smith RT, Lambros AM, Kuppe MK, Gruber SA. Recreational cannabis use impairs driving performance in the absence of acute intoxication. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2020;208:107771.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Hindocha C, Freeman TP, Xia JX, Shaban NDC, Curran HV. Acute memory and psychotomimetic effects of cannabis and tobacco both ‘joint’ and individually: a placebo-controlled trial. Psychol Med. 2017;47(15):2708–19.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Pope HG, Gruber AJ, Hudson JI, Huestis MA, Yurgelun-Todd D. Neuropsychological performance in long-term cannabis users. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58:909–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Courtney KE, Baca R, Doran N, Jacobson A, Liu TT, Jacobus J. The effects of nicotine and cannabis co-use during adolescence and young adulthood on white matter cerebral blood flow estimates. Psychopharmacology. 2020;237(12):3615–24.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Courtney KE, Sorg S, Baca R, Doran N, Jacobson A, Jacobus J. The effects of Nicotine and Cannabis Co-use during Late Adolescence on White Matter Fiber Tract microstructure. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2022;83:287–95.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Weintraub S, Dikmen SS, Heaton RK, Tulsky DS, Zelazo PD, Bauer PJ, et al. Cognition assessment using the NIH Toolbox. Neurology. 2013;80:S54–64.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Brown, Myers MG, Lippke K, Tapert SF, Stewart DG, Vik PW. Psychometric evaluation of the customary drinking and durg use record (CDDR): a measure of adolescent alcohol and drug involvement. J Stud Alcohol. 1998;59(4):427–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Karoly HC, Schacht JP, Meredith LR, Jacobus J, Tapert SF, Gray KM, et al. Investigating a novel fMRI cannabis cue reactivity task in youth. Addict Behav. 2019;89:20–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Karoly HC, Schacht JP, Jacobus J, Meredith LR, Taylor CT, Tapert SF, et al. Preliminary evidence that computerized approach avoidance training is not associated with changes in fMRI cannabis cue reactivity in non-treatment-seeking adolescent cannabis users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2019;200:145–52.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Jacobus J, Taylor CT, Gray KM, Meredith LR, Porter AM, Li I, et al. A multi-site proof-of-concept investigation of computerized approach-avoidance training in adolescent cannabis users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2018;187:195–204.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Meier MH, Caspi A, Ambler A, Harrington H, Houts R, Keefe RS, et al. Persistent cannabis users show neuropsychological decline from childhood to midlife. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109(40):E2657–64.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. 2021. Available from: https://www.R-project.org/.

  37. RStudio Team. RStudio: integrated development environment for R. Boston: RStudio, PBC; 2021.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Wade NE, Courtney KE, Doran N, Baca R, Aguinaldo LD, Thompson C, et al. Young adult E-Cigarette and combustible tobacco users attitudes, substance use behaviors, mental health, and neurocognitive performance. Brain Sci. 2022;12(7):889.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Tulsky DS, Carlozzi N, Chiaravalloti ND, Beaumont JL, Kisala PA, Mungas D, et al. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (NIHTB-CB): list sorting test to measure working memory. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2014;20(6):599–610.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Morie KP, Potenza MN. A Mini-review of relationships between Cannabis Use and neural foundations of reward Processing, Inhibitory Control and Working Memory. Front Psychiatry. 2021;12:657371.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Robledo-Menendez A, Vella M, Grandes P, Soria‐Gomez E. Cannabinoid control of hippocampal functions: the where matters. FEBS J. 2022;289:2162–75.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Iversen L. Cannabis and the brain. Brain. 2003;126:1252–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Dellazizzo L, Potvin S, Giguère S, Dumais A. Evidence on the acute and residual neurocognitive effects of cannabis use in adolescents and adults: a systematic meta-review of meta‐analyses. Addiction. 2022;117:1857–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Schoeler T, Bhattacharyya S. The effect of cannabis use on memory function: an update. Subst Abuse Rehabilitation. 2013;4:11–27.

    Google Scholar 

  45. ElSohly MA, Chandra S, Radwan M, Majumdar CG, Church JC. A Comprehensive Review of Cannabis Potency in the United States in the last decade. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2021;6(6):603–6.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Meredith LR, Lim AC, Ray LA. Neurocognitive performance in alcohol use disorder using the NIH toolbox: role of severity and sex differences. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2020;216:108269.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Gorey C, Kuhns L, Smaragdi E, Kroon E, Cousijn J. Age-related differences in the impact of cannabis use on the brain and cognition: a systematic review. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2019;269(1):37–58.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Crane NA, Schuster RM, Fusar-Poli P, Gonzalez R. Effects of cannabis on neurocognitive functioning: recent advances, neurodevelopmental influences, and sex differences. Neuropsychol Rev. 2013;23(2):117–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Schnakenberg Martin AM, D’Souza DC, Newman SD, Hetrick WP, O’Donnell BF. Differential cognitive performance in females and males with regular cannabis use. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2021;27(6):570–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute on Drug Abuse grants R01 DA054106 and R21 DA047953, and the California Tobacco-Related Disease Research Grants Program Office of the University of California Grants 580264 (PI: Jacobus), T30IP0962 (PI: Courtney), and by K08 DA050779 (PI: Wade).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

NEW designed the study, analyzed the data and wrote the first draft of the manuscript; KEW assisted in interpreting the results and substantially revising the manuscript; ALW and LH contributed to substantially revising the manuscript; JJ designed the study, contributed to the conception of the study, and substantially revised the manuscript. All authors have approvied the submitted version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Joanna Jacobus.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All aspects of the study protocol were approved by the UC San Diego IRB and all participants provided written informed consent.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wade, N.E., Courtney, K.E., Wallace, A.L. et al. Investigating sex differences and age of onset in emotion regulation, executive functioning, and cannabis use in adolescents and young adults. J Cannabis Res 6, 20 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s42238-024-00225-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s42238-024-00225-z

Keywords